Factors that Contribute to Successful Reading Comprehension

A compelling model of reading acquisition is that encapsulated in the ‘Simple View of Reading’ (Gough and Tunmer, 1986). This essentially describes the two central components of successful reading as being the capacity to read the words on the page accurately, and (simultaneously) the capacity to understand or comprehend the message or content being read.

In order to read accurately, students need to have grasped the alphabetic principle – that is, the knowledge that there is a predictable and reversible relationship between the sounds (phonemes) in the words we say, and the letters (graphemes) we use to write those words down. It is now widely accepted that the most effective approach to the teaching of accurate reading and spelling includes a multi-sensory, structured, synthetic phonics program, taught systematically, cumulatively and with ample opportunity for practise and application (NICHD, 2005; Rose, 2006; Hattie, 2009). As children’s reading develops they also need to be taught – and make use of – orthographic, morphological and semantic knowledge in order to read and spell accurately (Plaut, 2005). Unfortunately, the understanding of how best to teach reading comprehension has not been as extensively researched, or as well-documented, as the strategies recommended for the development of reading accuracy, despite its obvious importance.

What is reading comprehension?

In order to comprehend written material students need to:

  • Accurately read and understand the words being used (phonic knowledge and vocabulary);
  • Successfully link the units of meaning within each sentence (syntax);
  • Activate and integrate prior knowledge in order to make successful inferences (retrieve connected information from long term memory);
  • Successfully integrate the meaning of successive sentences (local coherence / working memory); and,
  • Establish how the whole text fits together (global coherence).

Comprehension is fundamentally the goal of both reading and listening. It enables readers and listeners to: acquire new knowledge and skills; experience and be aware of alternate ‘environments’; communicate successfully; and, achieve academically. The long-term goal of the reading teacher is to ensure that students learn to understand written language as well as they understand spoken language. It has been recognised that when children first start learning to read there is very little correlation between their language comprehension and their reading comprehension. However, once they are able to read accurately and fluently, there is a very strong correlation. The language of any text, whether it is spoken or written, is rarely explicit. In order to comprehend at a deeper level, readers need to make inferences that bridge elements of the text and support the coherence necessary for comprehension to occur.

Inference Making

The process of inference making allows readers to ‘fill the gaps’ in both spoken and written language and ensures that the text is understood at a deeper level. It allows readers to make sense of otherwise unconnected actions in a text (causal inferences) or may be used to elaborate or make predictions (elaborative inferences). Once students are reading fluently they tend to approximate the adult model of inference making (Perfetti, Landi and Oakhill, 2006). This is, in part, due to the fact that inference generation is considered to require high levels of cognitive processing. If a significant amount of processing is being dedicated to reading accuracy or semantic retrieval, then it is less likely to be utilised for inference making. It is also the case that less skilled readers are more likely to make inferences that support coherence than inferences that elaborate.
Essentially, to comprehend, readers need to make inferences by drawing on prior knowledge (including vocabulary knowledge) and experience. This means that individual differences in both knowledge and experience will impact on a person’s capacity to comprehend. However, deep understanding is likely to require the construction of meaning, not just passive retrieval of information (Kintsch and Rawson, 2005). For example, to understand a story, a student may need to infer the protagonist’s motivations or, to understand a philosophical argument, a reader may need to analyse the relationships between different aspects of the text.

Inference Making Difficulties

Four possible reasons that readers struggle to make inferences include:

  • Prior knowledge deficits (including poor vocabulary);
  • Poor understanding of syntax;
  • Apparent inability to know when to make inferences; and,
  • Processing limitations – eg. Poor working memory, attentional issues or language deficits.

Working memory deficits, in particular, are strongly correlated with poor reading comprehension (Gathercole & Alloway, 2008). Understanding a sentence involves remembering the words within the sentence, retrieving information from any preceding text, interpreting and connecting syntactic information, integrating prior knowledge, monitoring text coherence, and making and including inferences. All of these place high demand on working memory.

Assisting students to improve their comprehension

Students need to understand both the ‘bare bones’ of a sentence (literal meaning), often comprising a simple subject and predicate (eg. ‘The cow ate the grass’ or ‘The beautiful ballerina pirouetted gracefully across the stage.’) as well as higher-level factors such as: sensitivity to story structure; inference making; and, comprehension monitoring. As previously indicated, understanding the words in each sentence is exceptionally important. Children’s listening and reading comprehension has been shown to improve through the explicit teaching of vocabulary.

Once students have developed accurate and fluent reading, sentence comprehension strategies can be taught. Students learn to identify – what has happened, who was involved, when the event occurred, where the event took place, and why the event happened. This strategy serves as a foundation for later comprehension of longer text. Before they start utilising this approach, students need the ability to repeat five to seven word sentences, beginning with who and what sentences.

As part of their extensive and systematic review of early literacy teaching, the NRP (National Reading Panel) identified seven categories of effective evidence-based comprehension instruction:

  • Comprehension monitoring;
  • Cooperative learning;
  • Graphic and semantic organisers;/li>
  • Question answering (teachers model active questioning);
  • Question generation (students self-question to clarify understanding);
  • Story structure; and,
  • Summarisation.

The research suggests that explicitly teaching a combination of strategies to bolster comprehension is the most successful approach.

  1. Comprehension Monitoring

    Comprehension monitoring occurs when the reader is actively aware of whether he/she is understanding or remembering the text being processed. The two main components of comprehension monitoring are: ongoing monitoring and evaluating; and, implementing strategies when the text fails to ‘make sense’. It has been suggested that poor readers often read on automatically – unaware that something they have read ‘doesn’t make sense”. This is a strategy that can be started as early as pre-primary / year one, with students being taught how to ask themselves questions as they read (active reading) and monitor their understanding. For example – “Do I understand what I am reading?” “How does this connect to what I already know?” “What do I think might happen next?”

     

    When students pick up an inconsistency in the text they are reading (often the result of misreading a word or sentence) they need to know there are steps they can take to address this. Essentially they should: identify and isolate the difficulty; use ‘think-aloud’ strategies to highlight where and when the difficulty began; restate what was read; look back through the text; and/or, look forward in the text.

  2. Collaborative Learning

    Collaborative learning occurs when students work together in pairs or in small groups on structured activities – often with specific roles. For example in the Collaborative Strategic Reading program From Clunk to Click (Vaughn & Klinger, 1999) students are provided with cue sheets and work collaboratively through three phases of activity.

    1. Prior to reading they brainstorm in order to identify prior knowledge and predict what they will learn.
    2. During reading the students identify and discuss the parts of text that are difficult to understand, including any unfamiliar vocabulary. They also work at identifying the main focus (or ‘gist’) of the text by selecting the most important person, place or thing and then identifying the most important idea about that person, place or thing.
    3. After reading students review their findings. They identify some questions to check their understanding, and then determine whether they can answer those questions.
  3. Graphic and Semantic Organisers

    The value of graphic and semantic organisers is that they provide a visual representation of knowledge. This can be particularly valuable for students with poor working memory or with learning disabilities. Three important uses have been identified:

    1. They help students focus on text structure while reading;
    2. They provide tools to examine and visually represent textual relationships; and,
    3. They assist in the writing of well-structured summaries.

     

    Graphic organisers have been used for multiple purposes, including: generating lists of character traits; improving vocabulary; identifying relationships in expository texts; activating background knowledge; setting a purpose for reading; and, helping students to see the text structure. An important finding by the NRP in relation to the use of semantic and graphic organisers was the recognition of their capacity to activate prior knowledge. One strategy: K-W-L was identified as being particularly useful.

    1. K – Identifying what I already Know.
    2. W – Deciding what I Want to learn (setting a purpose for reading / including determining what new information will be gathered).
    3. L – Recalling what I Learned from my reading.

     

     

  4. Question Answering

    Students need to analyse the types of questions being asked, and consider both the text and their prior knowledge, in order to successfully formulate answers to questions being asked (including self-generated questions). In the Questions-Answers Relationships (QAR) program (Marzola, 2011), students are taught two major categories of question-answer relationships: ‘in the book’ or ‘in my head’. Readers need to refer to the text to answer ‘in the book’ questions but need to explore further, drawing on prior knowledge, to answer ‘in my head’ questions. Essentially through strategies like the QAR program, students learn to identify different types of questions and also develop different strategies to answer the questions being asked.

    This allows students to discriminate between those questions that can be answered by referring back to the text (literal and inferential) and those that require skills in higher-level thinking (critical analysis, interpretation, generalisation and extension of ideas from text). Students need practice in answering a range of question types and to understand the difference between them.

  5. Question Generating

    Teaching students strategies to improve their capacity to generate questions as they read is important and those teachers who model appropriate questions before, during and after reading have been found to have a positive impact on their students’ comprehension of text.

    In the NRP’s review of the research into successful reading comprehension instruction it was suggested that there was “the strongest scientific evidence available…. for the effectiveness of asking readers to generate questions during reading” (NICHD, 2000, p 45). Used in isolation, it was found to have the strongest impact of the top seven strategies identified. Self-questioning has been found to distinguish good readers from poor readers in that good readers ask themselves questions before, during and after reading, whereas poor readers often fail to self-question.

  6. Story Structure

    Many of the texts introduced in the primary school setting are stories, the structure of which tend to follow a predictable format involving characters, setting, problem, goal, action, and outcome (or resolution of the problem). When students are taught about story structure, they become more capable of retelling stories within a logical framework. They also show improvements in asking and answering who, what, when, where, why, and how questions.

    For those students who struggle to recall the classic story structure, including those with working memory problems, additional strategies can be introduced (Marzola, 2005). For example:

    1. Five finger recall – each finger is used as a memory prompt for one of the structural elements – characters, setting, problem, plot, resolution.
    2. STORE – a mnemonic used to assist students in the retelling of stories – Setting (who, what, when, where);
      Trouble (what is the trouble / problem to be solved?);
      Order of action (what happened to solve the problem?);
      Resolution (what was the outcome (resolution) for each action?); and, End (what happened in the end?).
    3. The Language Circle – the five structural elements are written on Post-it notes and the student flags each of the story elements in the text as it is read.

 

Summarisation

There are a number of tasks involved in successful summarising:

  1. Deciding what are the most important ideas in the text;
  2. Generalising from examples or from ideas / things that are repeated; and,
  3. Ignoring irrelevant details.

Students with poor reading skills or who have learning disabilities often struggle to summarise text successfully. A strategy developed by a team at the University of Kansas to assist students experiencing difficulties with summarising is known as the RAP Paraphrasing Strategy. It has three specific steps:

  1. Read a paragraph.
  2. Ask yourself – “What were the main idea and details in this paragraph?”
  3. Put the main idea and details into your own words.

In order to locate the main idea and details it is useful to remind students that the first and/or last sentence, key vocabulary, and repetitions of the same word or words often relate to the main idea.

An approach that brings together many of the strategies identified above is the Reciprocal Teaching (RT) approach (Palincsar & Brown, 1984). Essentially this is an instructional activity that involves a structured dialogue between the teacher and the students incorporating the strategies of: clarifying, question generating, summarising and predicting. Initially the activities are highly scaffolded but as the students master the strategies, the input of the teacher is reduced.

Current Research

An ongoing project undertaken by the University of York (UK) entitled the Reading for Meaning Project: Evaluating interventions designed to support reading comprehension has set out to examine three approaches thought to improve reading comprehension in poorly performing children;

  • An intervention program incorporating oral language activities (the Oral Language (OL) program);
  • An intervention program incorporating written language activities (the Text Comprehension (TC) Program);
  • An intervention program incorporating both oral and written language activities (the Combined (COM) Program).

The effectiveness of the interventions has been evaluated using the WIAT II (Weschler Individual Achievement Test) and compared to students in classes where the teaching approach had remained unchanged. The results indicated that all intervention groups improved significantly in comparison to the control groups and that these gains were maintained over time. However, the Oral Language intervention group achieved the greatest gains beyond the intervention period.

The Oral Language program focused on the development of the students’ understanding and production of oral language and used a listening version of the Reciprocal Teaching approach to target vocabulary, figurative language and spoken narrative.

For more information about this research visit the Wiley online store.

Final Comments

As indicated at the beginning of this article, comprehension is the goal of both reading and listening. The fact that many students struggle to develop adequate reading comprehension skills is of major concern, as is the lack of information available to teachers on evidence-based intervention. It is hoped that this snapshot of some of the teaching strategies known to have an impact on student outcomes proves to be of value to teachers looking for effective comprehension strategies.

Mandy Nayton
Executive Officer
DSF Literacy and Clinical Services

This article was taken from DSF Bulletin – Vol. 38 (Spring 2013). DSF Members receive copies and can access back-issues of the DSF Bulletin

References:

Clarke, P., Snowling, M., Truelove, E., and Hulme, C. Ameliorating Children’s Reading Comprehension Difficulties: A Randomized Controlled Trial, Psychological Science, (2010), 21, pp 1106–1115.
Gathercole, S & Alloway, T. (2008), Working memory and learning: A Practical Guide for Teachers, Sage Publications, London.
Gough, P. B. & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, (7), pp 6-10.
Hattie, J., (2009), Visible Learning: A synthesis of over 800 meta-analyses of achievement, Routledge, Oxon.
Kintsch, W. and Rawson, K.A. (2005), Comprehension. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook . pp 209-226 Oxford UK: Blackwell publishing.
Marzola, E., (2011), Strategies to improve reading comprehension in the multi-sensory classroom. In Wolf, B. & Birsh, J. Multisensory Teaching of Basic Language Skills, Paul Brookes, Baltimore.
Nation, K. (2005), Children’s Reading Comprehension Difficulties, In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook . pp 248-266 Oxford UK: Blackwell publishing.
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), (2005), Report of the National Reading panel. Teaching children to read. Available on-line at http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/nrp/report.htm
Palincsar, A.S & brown, A. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, pp 117-175
Perfetti, C., Landi, N. & Oakhill, J. (2005), The Acquisition of Reading Comprehension Skill, In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook . pp 228-247 Oxford UK: Blackwell publishing.
Plaut, D. C. (2005). Connectionist approaches to reading. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook . pp 24-38 Oxford UK: Blackwell publishing.
Rose, J. (2006), Independent Review of the Teaching of Early Reading, Department for Education and Skills.
Stahl, K. (2004), Proof, Practice and Promise: Comprehension strategy instruction in the primary grades. The Reading teacher, 57 (1), pp 598-609
Vaughn, S. & Klinger, J.K. (1999). Teaching reading comprehension through Collaborative Strategic Reading. Intervention in School and Clinic. (34)5, pp.284-292.

 

This entry was posted in News, Uncategorized. Bookmark the permalink.

Comments are closed.